Archives for November 14, 2017
Qualitative Sampling
Study Notes:
Qualitative Research: Sampling & Sample Size Considerations
Adapted from a presentation by Dr. Bonnie Nastasi,
Director of School Psychology Program
Sampling for Qualitative Research
Sampling, as it relates to research, refers to the selection of individuals, units, and/or settings to be studied. Whereas quantitative studies strive for random sampling, qualitative studies often use purposeful or criterion-based sampling, that is, a sample that has the characteristics relevant to the research question(s). For example, if you are interested in studying adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse, interviewing a random sample of 10 people may yield only one adult survivor, thus, you will essentially have a sample size of one and need to continue to randomly sample people until you have interviewed an appropriate number of who have survived childhood sexual abuse. This is not a wise use of your time.
The difference in sampling strategies between quantitative and qualitative studies is due to the different goals of each research approach. Recall that typical quantitative research seeks to infer from a sample to a population (for example, a relationship or a treatment effect). In general, you want to include a variety of types of people in a quantitative study so that it generalizes beyond those in your study. Thus, the goal of quantitative approaches can be stated as, ”empirical generalization to many.”
Qualitative research, on the other hand, typically starts with a specific group, type of individual, event, or process. As in the qualitative study of adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse example above, you would choose your sample very purposefully and include in your study only those with this particular experience. The goal of qualitative research can be stated as “in-depth understanding.”
It is true that some aspects of quantitative sampling could be relevant to a qualitative researcher. For example, if you are interested in children’s experiences of Hurricane Katrina and you have access to 3,000 school children, all of whom experienced the hurricane, you might choose to randomly sample 10 children from the 3,000 for your qualitative study. In the case of ethnographic survey research, you might even seek to obtain sample sizes similar to those in a quantitative design. It could be said, then, that there are more ambiguities than “rules” when it comes to qualitative research in general and that choosing a sampling strategy and sample size for qualitative research is no different. What is important to remember is that the strategy you adopt will be driven by the:
- Research question(s)/purpose
- Time frame of your study
- Resources available
Following is a list of common sampling strategies. As you read these strategies, think of which would be most relevant for your area of interest. In many cases, you will see ways to combine the strategies to create an effective approach. For example, you may use snowball sampling as a method to identify a set of extreme/deviant cases. This is an example of combination or mixed purposeful sampling. Thus these methods are not mutually exclusive; a research design may adopt a range of strategies.
Common Qualitative Sampling Strategies [1]
- Extreme or Deviant Case Sampling—Looks at highly unusual manifestations of the phenomenon of interest, such as outstanding success/notable failures, top of the class/dropouts, exotic events, crises. This strategy tries to select particular cases that would glean the most information, given the research question. One example of an extreme/deviant case related to battered women would be battered women who kill their abusers.
- Intensity Sampling—Chooses information-rich cases that manifest the phenomenon intensely, but not extremely, such as good students/poor students, above average/below average. This strategy is very similar to extreme/deviant case sampling as it uses the same logic. The difference is that the cases selected are not as extreme. This type of sampling requires that you have prior information on the variation of the phenomena under study so that you can choose intense, although not extreme, examples. For example, heuristic research uses the intense, personal experience(s) of the researcher. If one were studying jealousy, you would need to have had an intense experience with this particular emotion; a mild or pathologically extreme experience would not likely elucidate the phenomena in the same way as an intense experience.
- Maximum Variation Sampling—Selects a wide range of variation on dimensions of interest. The purpose is to discover/uncover central themes, core elements, and/or shared dimensions that cut across a diverse sample while at the same time offering the opportunity to document unique or diverse variations. For example, to implement this strategy, you might create a matrix (of communities, people, etc.) where each item on the matrix is as different (on relevant dimensions) as possible from all other items.
- Homogeneous Sampling—Brings together people of similar backgrounds and experiences. It reduces variation, simplifies analysis, and facilitates group interviewing. This strategy is used most often when conducting focus groups. For example, if you are studying participation in a parenting program, you might sample all single-parent, female head of households.
- Typical Case Sampling—Focuses on what is typical, normal, and/or average. This strategy may be adopted when one needs to present a qualitative profile of one or more typical cases. When using this strategy you must have a broad consensus about what is “average.” For example, if you were working to begin development projects in Third World countries, you might conduct a typical case sampling of “average” villages. Such a study would uncover critical issues to be addressed for most villages by looking at the ones you sampled.
- Critical Case Sampling—Looks at cases that will produce critical information. In order to use this method, you must know what constitutes a critical case. This method permits logical generalization and maximum application of information to other cases because if it’s true of this one case, it’s likely to be true of all other case. For example, if you want to know if people understand a particular set of federal regulations, you may present the regulations to a group of highly educated people (“If they can’t understand them, then most people probably cannot”) and/or you might present them to a group of under-educated people (“If they can understand them, then most people probably can”).
- Snowball or Chain Sampling—Identifies cases of interest from people who know people who know what cases are information-rich, that is, who would be a good interview participant. Thus, this is an approach used for locating information-rich cases. You would begin by asking relevant people something like: “Who knows a lot about ___?” For example, you would ask for nominations, until the nominations snowball, getting bigger and bigger. Eventually, there should be a few key names that are mentioned repeatedly.
- Criterion Sampling—Selects all cases that meet some criterion. This strategy is typically applied when considering quality assurance issues. In essence, you choose cases that are information-rich and that might reveal a major system weakness that could be improved. For example, if the average length of stay for a certain surgical procedure is three days, you might set a criterion for being in the study as anyone whose stay exceeded three days. Interviewing these cases may offer information related to aspects of the process/system that could be improved.
- Theory-Based or Operational Construct or Theoretical Sampling—dentifies manifestations of a theoretical construct of interest so as to elaborate and examine the construct. This strategy is similar to criterion sampling, except it is more conceptually focused. This strategy is used in grounded theory studies. You would sample people/incidents, etc., based on whether or not they manifest/represent an important theoretical or operational construct. For example, if you were interested in studying the theory of “resiliency” in adults who were physically abused as children, you would sample people who meet theory-driven criteria for “resiliency.”
- Confirming and Disconfirming Sampling—Seeks cases that are both “expected” and the “exception” to what is expected. In this way, this strategy deepens initial analysis, seeks exceptions, and tests variation. In this strategy you find both confirming cases (those that add depth, richness, credibility) as well as disconfirming cases (example that do not fit and are the source of rival interpretations). This strategy is typically adopted after initial fieldwork has established what a confirming case would be. For example, if you are studying certain negative academic outcomes related to environmental factors, like low SES, low parental involvement, high teacher to student ratios, lack of funding for a school, etc. you would look for both confirming cases (cases that evidence the negative impact of these factors on academic performance) and disconfirming cases (cases where there is no apparent negative association between these factors and academic performance).
- Stratified Purposeful Sampling—Focuses on characteristics of particular subgroups of interest; facilitates comparisons. This strategy is similar to stratified random sampling (samples are taken within samples), except the sample size is typically much smaller. In stratified sampling you “stratify” a sample based on a characteristic. Thus, if you are studying academic performance, you would sample a group of below average performers, average performers, and above average performers. The main goal of this strategy is to capture major variations (although common themes may emerge).
- Opportunistic or Emergent Sampling—Follows new leads during fieldwork, takes advantage of the unexpected, and is flexible. This strategy takes advantage of whatever unfolds as it is unfolding, and may be used after fieldwork has begun and as a researcher becomes open to sampling a group or person they may not have initially planned to interview. For example, you might be studying 6th grade students’ awareness of a topic and realize you will gain additional understanding by including 5th grade students’ as well.
- Purposeful Random Sampling—Looks at a random sample. This strategy adds credibility to a sample when the potential purposeful sample is larger than one can handle. While this is a type of random sampling, it uses small sample sizes, thus the goal is credibility, not representativeness or the ability to generalize. For example, if you want to study clients at a drug rehabilitation program, you may randomly select 10 of 300 current cases to follow. This reduces judgment within a purposeful category, because the cases are picked randomly and without regard to the program outcome.
- Sampling Politically Important Cases—Seeks cases that will increase the usefulness and relevance of information gained based on the politics of the moment. This strategy attracts attention to the study (or avoids attracting undesired attention by purposefully eliminating from the sample politically sensitive cases). This strategy is a variation on critical case sampling. For example, when studying voter behavior, one might choose the 2000 election, not only because it would provide insight, but also because it would likely attract attention.
- Convenience Sampling—Selects cases based on ease of accessibility. This strategy saves time, money, and effort, however, has the weakest rationale along with the lowest credibility. This strategy may yield information-poor cases because cases are picked simply because they are easy to access, rather than on a specific strategy/rationale. Sampling your co-workers, family members or neighbors simply because they are “there” is an example of convenience sampling.
- Combination or Mixed Purposeful Sampling—Combines two or more strategies listed above. Basically, using more than one strategy above is considered combination or mixed purposeful sampling. This type of sampling meets multiple interests and needs. For example, you might use chain sampling in order to identify extreme or deviant cases. That is, you might ask people to identify cases that would be considered extreme/deviant and do this until you have consensus on a set of cases that you would sample.
Sample Sizes: Considerations
When determining sample size for qualitative studies, it is important to remember that there are no hard and fast rules. There are, however, at least two considerations:
- What sample size will reach saturation or redundancy? That is, how large does the sample need to be to allow for the identification of consistent patterns? Some researchers say the size of the sample should be large enough to leave you with “nothing left to learn.” In other words, you might conduct interviews, and after the tenth one, realize that there are no new concepts emerging. That is, the concepts, themes, etc. begin to be redundant.
- How large a sample is needed to represent the variation within target population? That is, how large must a sample be to in order to assess an appropriate amount of diversity or variation that is represented in the population of interest?
You may estimate sample size, based on the approach of the study or the data collection method used. For each category there are some related rules of thumb, represented in the tables below.
Rules of Thumb Based on Approach:
Research Approach | Rule of Thumb |
Biography/Case Study | Select one case or one person. |
Phenomenology | Assess 10 people. If you reach saturation prior to assessing ten people you may use fewer. |
Grounded theory/ethnography/action research | Assess 20-30 people, which typically is enough to reach saturation. |
Rules of Thumb Based on Data Collection Method:
Data Collection Method | Rule of Thumb |
Interviewing key informants | Interview approximately five people. |
In-depth interviews
|
Interview approximately 30 people. |
Focus groups
|
Create groups that average 5-10 people each. In addition, consider the number of focus groups you need based on “groupings” represented in the research question. That is, when studying males and females of three different age groupings, plan for six focus groups, giving you one for each gender and three age groups for each gender. |
Ethnographic surveys
|
Select a large and representative sample (purposeful or random based on purpose) with numbers similar to those in a quantitative study. |
There should also be consideration of the size of a good database: one that will yield data that are of sufficient quality and quantity. While the quality of the data is impacted by the quality of the interview protocol, the quantity of data is also a factor. For example, with a well conceived interview protocol, a 10-20 hour database should provide enough data to support a solid qualitative dissertation. In this case, the following chart can be used:
Guidelines for Length of Interviews:
Number of Interviews | Length of each interview |
10 | 1 – 2 hours |
20 | 30 minutes – 1 hour |
30 | 20 – 40 minutes |
Adjustments may be made if there are other forms of qualitative data collection involved. For example, if there is a 2 -hour focus group and 10 interviews, the duration of the interviews might be shortened.
Conclusion
Regardless of the strategy or strategies you adopt for a study, and/or the sample size you plan for, you need to provide a rationale for your choices by articulating the expected benefits and weaknesses of any strategy/sample size you choose. A key component of any qualitative research design is flexibility. Accordingly, if you choose a qualitative research design, you must have high tolerance for ambiguity.
References
Camic, P. M, Rhodes, J. E., & & Yardley, L. (Ed.). (2003). Qualitative research in psychology: Expanding perspectives in methodology and design. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Creswell, J. W. (1998). Qualitative Inquiry & Research Design: Choosing Among Five Traditions. Thousand Oaks: CA. Sag Publications, Inc.
Dey, I. (1999). Grounding grounded theory: Guidelines for qualitative inquiry. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Harter, S. (1978). Effectance motivation reconsidered: Toward a developmental model. Human Development, 21, 34–64.
Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. New York: Guilford.
Hitchcock, J. H, Nastasi, B. K., Dai, D. C., Newman, J., Jayasena, A., Bernstein-Moore, R., Sarkar, S., & Varjas, K. (2004). Illustrating a mixed-method approach for identifying and validating culturally specific constructs. Accepted for publication in Journal of School Psychology.
Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Thousand Oaks: CA. Sage Publications, Inc.
Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative data analysis (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
Nastasi, B.K., Moore, R. B., & Varjas, K. M. (2004). School-Based Mental Health Services: Creating Comprehensive and Culturally Specific Programs. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Nastasi, B. K., Varjas, K., Sarkar, S., & Jayasena, A. (1998). Participatory model of mental health programming: Lessons learned from work in a developing country. School Psychology Review, 27 (2), 260–276.
Patton, M. Q. (2001). Qualitative evaluation and research methods (3rd ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
Sarkar, S. (2003). Gender as a cultural factor influencing mental health among adolescent students in India and Sri Lanka. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University at Albany, SUNY.
Schensul, J. J., & LeCompte, M. D. (Eds.). (1999). Ethnographer’s toolkit: Volumes 1–7. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.
Varjas, K. M. (2003). A participatory culture-specific consultation (PCSC) approach to intervention development. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University at Albany, SUNY.
Wolcott, H. F. (1990). Writing up qualitative research. Thousand Oaks: CA. Sage Publications, Inc.
[1] Patton, M. Q. (2001). Qualitative evaluation and research methods (3rd ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Recent Comments